Negative intergroup contact is more influential, but positive intergroup contact is more common: Assessing contact prominence and contact prevalence in five Central European countries
Abstract
The present research tested the idea that the ecological impact of intergroup contact on outgroup attitudes can be fully understood only when relative frequency and relative influence of positive and negative contact are considered simultaneously. Participants from five European countries (Austria, the Czech Republic, Germany, Poland and Slovakia; N = 1276) freely described their contact experiences with people of neighboring nationalities and then reported on their outgroup attitudes. Contact descriptions were coded for positive versus negative valence and for person versus situation framing. Consistently across the five participant groups, positive intergroup contact was reported to occur three times more frequently than negative intergroup contact; however, positive contact was found to be only weakly related to outgroup attitudes. On the contrary, the less frequent negative (vs. positive) contact was comparatively more influential in shaping outgroup attitudes, especially when negativity was reported around the contact person, rather than the contact situation. This research's findings reconcile contrasting lines of past research on intergroup contact and suggest that the greater prevalence of positive contact may compensate for the greater prominence of negative contact, thus leading to modest net improvements in outgroup attitudes after intergroup contact. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
… good may prevail over bad by superior force of numbers
(Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001, p. 323)
A large body of research shows that intergroup contact typically reduces prejudice (for a meta‐analysis, see Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Recent comparisons of positive versus negative intergroup contact, however, found that negative contact is more influential in shaping outgroup attitudes than positive contact (Barlow et al., 2012; Dhont & Van Hiel, 2009). This leads to an important question: If negative contact has a stronger influence on outgroup attitudes than positive contact, why does intergroup contact ultimately reduce, rather than exacerbate, intergroup animosity? We believe that Baumeister and colleagues' (2001) simple and elegant idea provides the answer: While negative contact is more influential, positive contact may outweigh its influence “by superior force of numbers”, in other words by simply being more common.
In this research, we compared the relative influence, or strength of association between outgroup attitudes and positive versus negative contact experiences, as well as examined the relative frequency of positive and negative contact in real life settings. We argue that the contradictory findings of past research can be reconciled and a fuller understanding of intergroup contact achieved by simultaneously considering both frequency and size of effects on attitudes of positive and negative intergroup contact.
Positive versus Negative Intergroup Contact
A large meta‐analysis of 515 studies with 713 independent samples found that intergroup contact typically reduces prejudice (mean r = −.21; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). A closer examination of the studies' design also revealed that the contact–prejudice link was further enhanced when the contact experience was structured in line with Allport's (1954) propositions for optimal contact (i.e., equal status, common goals, intergroup cooperation and institutional support; mean r = −.29). Studies that did not attempt to optimally structure the contact situation yielded comparatively smaller but nonetheless significant effects (mean r = −.20; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2011). Hence, Pettigrew and Tropp drew the general optimistic conclusion that intergroup contact is beneficial not only under carefully controlled conditions of the psychology laboratory but also in real‐world settings where Allport's optimal conditions are less likely to be met.
Pettigrew and Tropp (2006, 2011; see also Pettigrew, 2008), nevertheless, admitted that past contact research in general—and, as a result, their synthesis—suffered from a neglect of negative intergroup contact. Consequently, the effects of positive contact found in past studies could not be compared with the (missing) effects of negative contact. Critically, positive contact represents only one part of the full evaluative spectrum of possible contact experiences (Paolini, Harwood, & Rubin, 2010; Stark, Flache, & Veenstra, 2013). Without considering the effects of negative contact on outgroup attitudes, part of the picture is absent, and this poses a serious limitation to the knowledge gained from intergroup contact research and its applicability to real life settings (for a similar point, see Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006).
One of the first investigations to explicitly compare the relative effects of positive and negative contact was conducted by Paolini and colleagues (2010; see also Paolini, Harwood et al., 2014). Their research found experimental and longitudinal evidence that the awareness of group memberships is higher during negative than positive intergroup contact. Drawing from extensive evidence synthesized by Brown and Hewstone (2005), Paolini et al. predicted that because of heightened category salience, negative contact experiences with specific outgroup members should generalize to the outgroup as a whole more readily than positive contact experiences. Tests of this idea by Barlow and colleagues (2012) recently confirmed that negative contact is indeed more influential or “prominent” in shaping outgroup attitudes than positive contact. Consistent with Paolini et al.'s premises, Barlow et al. found across six independent Australian and American samples that the prejudice‐increasing effects of negative contact were larger than the prejudice‐reducing effects of positive contact. Similarly, in a general community sample from Belgium, Dhont and Van Hiel (2009) found that the effect of negative contact with immigrants on worsening racism by far outweighed the effect of positive contact on racism reduction (r = .46, p < .001 vs. r = .28, p < .01, respectively).
Overall, this second line of research offers a less optimistic conclusion than Pettigrew and Tropp's (2006) meta‐analysis: It suggests that intergroup contact may be naturally skewed towards enhancing rather than reducing intergroup animosity, because of the disproportionate influence of negative contact on category salience and on outgroup attitudes. Other recent studies directly comparing the effects of positive and negative contact have returned a less consistent picture. Contrary to Paolini et al. and Barlow et al., Stark and colleagues (2013) found that in the classroom context, positive and negative attitudes towards particular outgroup students generalized to outgroup attitudes to the same degree (see also Paolini, McIntyre, & Hewstone, 2014). Another study has shown a relative prominence of positive contact (Pettigrew, 2008; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2011, Chapter 12). Hence, although there is some evidence that negative contact is a stronger predictor of outgroup attitudes, there is also evidence of null or even opposite effects.
More research is needed in this area before definitive conclusions about this prominence issue can be reached. One aim of the present research was to contribute to this effort. A second, key aim was to provide a fuller picture of positive–negative effects of intergroup contact that considers not only their relative influence on outgroup attitudes but also their relative frequency in naturalistic settings.
Strengths and Weaknesses of Past Contact Research
In designing the present research, we took into account both strengths and weaknesses of previous studies. We stressed already how Pettigrew and Tropp's (2006) optimistic conclusions reflect an impressively large pool of studies in which negative contact is visibly underrepresented. An undeniable strength of Pettigrew and Tropp's work, however, is its careful consideration of the rigor of the research study designs, target groups and participants' characteristics. Critically for the issues at stake here, its elaborate methodology controlled for a wide range of biases, including the causal sequence problem, and the file drawer issue, which can significantly distort the representativeness of the studies sampled for a meta‐analysis. Such high levels of control, coupled with the absolute size of their study sample, thus raise the possibility that Pettigrew and Tropp's positively skewed study sample accurately reflects the disproportionately larger frequency of positive (vs. negative) contact in people's ordinary experiences in real life settings. In other words, positive contact might be only modestly influential and comparatively less influential than negative contact for outgroup attitudes. However, the greater prevalence of positive contact would ultimately translate into the positive balance for intergroup contact effects as for the meta‐analysis' central finding. We explored this possibility further in the present research.
In contrast, the experimental work of Paolini et al. (2010, 2014) explicitly compared and contrasted the effects of positive and negative contact by assigning an equal number of participants to either one positive or one negative contact condition. While this experimental approach allows firm conclusions about direction of causality (cf. extant correlational evidence as reviewed in Paolini et al., 2010), it obviously lacks ecological validity. Firstly, because it implies an idealized view of either “purely positive” or “purely negative” contact that may be rare in naturalistic settings. Secondly, because the experimental procedure forces a balanced, equal frequency for positive and negative contact that may inaccurately map onto the ecology of intergroup contact in real life settings. As a result, this type of research neglects possible differences in the natural occurrence of positive/negative experiences in everyday intergroup encounters.
Some studies have recently started to explore the frequency of participants' past positive and negative contact (Study 2 of Barlow et al., 2012; Dhont & Van Hiel, 2009; Pettigrew, 2008; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2011, Chapter 12) and have reported a relative prevalence of positive over negative contact. All of these past studies, however, have employed explicit questions about contact valence. These methods require participants' retrospective awareness of the frequency of positive and negative events that might be either inaccessible or inaccurate (for an extensive discussion of biases in retrospective valence appraisals, see Schwarz, 2007). Furthermore, overt questions about positive and negative contact experiences lack validity because (i) they force respondents into evaluations of past contact they might otherwise spontaneously not articulate and (ii) they prime respondents with expectations that past contact should be either positive or negative. By explicitly probing considerations of contact valence, overt measures bear the risk of activating social desirability concerns and lead to biased valence appraisals.
Towards a More Ecologically Valid Analysis of Contact Prevalence and Prominence
Our research aimed to reconcile the opposing outlooks on intergroup contact of past studies by advancing an analysis that encompasses both frequency and influence of positive versus negative contact on outgroup attitudes. We offset the measurement limitations of previous studies by using an unobtrusive, free‐response approach to the assessment of contact valence. To get closer to the reality of multifaceted intergroup encounters, we asked participants to freely describe their own experience with outgroup members without any valence probes. Consequently, contact descriptions could mention exclusively positive, exclusively negative, both positive and negative or no valence at all. If participants included evaluative assessments in their contact descriptions, independent judges coded them along a positivity dimension and a negativity dimension; we regarded these evaluative assessments as genuine appraisals of contact valence, rather than methodological artifacts. Also, in contrast to a focus on within participants' retrospective estimates of frequency for positive and negative experiences in past research (e.g., Barlow et al., 2012; Dhont & Van Hiel, 2009; Pettigrew, 2008), in our research, we focused on relative frequencies of reports of positive and negative contact across participants—that is, within the overall sample of contact descriptions and away from participants' gross frequency estimates. Linking of past positive and negative contact experiences with participants' evaluations of the outgroup enabled us to compare the relative influence of positive and negative contact on outgroup attitudes.
A unique advantage of this free‐response approach is that it allowed us to obtain not only evaluative but also non‐evaluative accounts of past contact experiences. In ordinary daily life, people may not necessarily evaluate all of their experiences. At the same time, the absence of valence in the retrieval of past contact does not necessarily mean that contact has no effect on outgroup attitudes (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). This important feature in the ecology of people's ordinary experience has been neglected in past investigations. In our prevalence analyses, the inclusion of non‐evaluative intergroup contact experiences contributed to a fuller pool of participants' accounts of past experiences with outgroup members. In our prominence analyses (i.e., the analysis of the size of the contact–attitudes effects), outgroup attitudes associated with non‐evaluative past contact served as a benchmark against which outgroup attitudes associated with positive versus negative contact were assessed. Overall, this inclusion of non‐evaluative contact in our analysis—we believe—contributes to make the present research into a more ecological examination.
In this study, we treated contact valence in two distinct ways. In our assessment of frequency and size of effects, we first focused on exclusively positive and exclusively negative contact and directly compared these two types of evaluative accounts. This first approach replicates the usual way in which contact is treated in past experimental studies on valence asymmetries (i.e., contact is either positive or negative). In a second, more ecologically valid approach to contact valence, we assessed both contact positivity and contact negativity separately and simultaneously—for these analyses, we contrasted the presence of positively and negatively valenced contact to their absence. This second approach allowed us to account for maximum variability in past intergroup experiences.
The Role of Person versus Situation Framing of Intergroup Contact
In the contact literature, the outgroup interaction partner is explicitly or implicitly regarded as the key carrier of the effect of intergroup contact on outgroup attitudes (Stark et al., 2013; see, e.g., Brown & Hewstone, 2005, for an extensive discussion of individual‐to‐group generalization). Here, we argue that there is more to intergroup contact than just people of different group memberships and the building of evaluative group judgments might rely on a multitude of sources. In the present research, we extended our analysis to valence appraisals that are framed around the situation or context in which contact takes place. Intergroup contact scholars have been as much as necessary preoccupied with situational conditions that moderate the effect of contact on attitudes (see Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006); however, they have not directly compared the effects of valence associated with the contact partners versus the effect of valence associated with the situational context. Although the intergroup contact literature does not assist in spelling out how person versus situation framing of contact may influence outgroup attitudes, we can make reasonable inferences from well‐established attribution literature.
The person–situation dichotomy is central to attribution research. This expansive literature indicates that at least in Western societies, individuals spontaneously prefer person‐based, over situation‐based, explanations of behavior (e.g., Choi, Nisbett, & Norenzayan, 1999; Ross & Nisbett, 1991). In the intergroup domain, this relative attribution preference is referred to as the “ultimate attribution error” (Hewstone, 1989; Pettigrew, 1979). Person‐based attributions have been found to be more influential bases for group judgments in the stereotyping literature. For example, Wilder, Simon, and Faith (1996) demonstrated that a single outgroup member was able to change outgroup stereotypes when his or her counterstereotypical behavior was attributed to stable dispositional causes. The very same counterstereotypical behavior had no effect on group stereotyping when it was attributed to external causes.
We expected a similar person–situation asymmetry to hold in intergroup contact effects. In this research, we isolated the novel and unique contribution (if any) that the valence of the contact situation may exert on outgroup attitudes. Moreover, we contrasted the relative strength of associations between outgroup attitudes and valence framed around the contact person to that of valence framed around the contact situation. For this purpose, our participants were let free to frame their contact description around the contact partner(s) (person framing) or more impersonally (situation framing); the independent judges simply coded each contact description along these two separate dimensions using a reliable coding protocol.
Drawing from this extant attribution literature, we expected person framings to be generally more prevalent and prominent than situation framings. If person framing of behavior is indeed a more spontaneous and natural option in Western societies, person framings of contact should be relatively more frequent than situation framings. Person framing of contact should also have a stronger influence on attitudes. Critically, we were in a position to explore possible interactions between the effects of positive/negative contact valence discussed earlier and this person/situation‐framing dichotomy. Given the prime role of the person (vs. situation) in interpretations of past experiences and the greater salience of negative (vs. positive) contact (Paolini et al., 2010; Paolini, Harwood et al., 2014), we expected that negative contact framed around the contact person (while not necessarily most frequent) would be most influential in shaping outgroup attitudes.
Summary of Key Predictions and Overview of the Research Setting
The present study investigated how frequency and influence of positive and negative intergroup contact accounts operate together in determining outgroup attitudes. In the spirit of Baumeister et al.'s earlier quote, we tested the hypotheses that positive contact is more frequent in people's ordinary experiences of intergroup contact but is comparatively less influential for outgroup attitudes and that negative contact is more influential for outgroup attitudes but less frequent. We expected valence asymmetries in the size of the effects of negative (vs. positive) contact to be particularly pronounced in person‐framed (vs. situation‐framed) descriptions of intergroup contact. As discussed early, the key novelty of our study design rests in its unobtrusive measures of contact valence and contact framing, soliciting open‐ended and unconstrained descriptions of past contact.
We tested our hypotheses among real‐world groups and in a real‐world context, investigating intergroup contact between the nationals of five different countries in Central Europe: Austria, the Czech Republic, Germany, Poland and Slovakia—geographical locales that have been significantly underrepresented in past contact research (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Our research focused on intergroup contact in border regions, a setting not well studied by social psychologists, although widely featured in sociological and especially anthropological literature (Alvarez, 1995; Kohli, 2000; Wilson & Donnan, 1998). According to anthropologists, people living in border regions have more occasions and need to reformulate and establish their national identity in the face of ever‐present outgroups (Wilson, 2012). We targeted participants in border regions because border regions provide greater opportunities for cross‐national contact as compared with central regions.
Although there are no open conflicts in Central Europe, intergroup tensions and pattern of inequalities based on historical, economical and language grounds burden the relationships between these Germanic and Slavic countries. From a historical perspective, Germany has been negatively associated with the events of the Second World War in the eyes of their eastern neighbors (e.g., these attitudes resulted in forceful expulsions of autochthon German‐speaking residents from their homes in what was then called Czechoslovakia during the WWII aftermath). The difficult relations between Germanic and Slavic countries were further compounded by the Czech Republic, Slovakia and Poland being subjected to Russian influence and being cut off from their western neighbors for over 40 years by the Iron Curtain. More recent patterns of economic affluence by Germany and Austria have sustained mutual aloofness in contact between the nationals of these five Central European countries; the historically ingrained separation between Germanic and Slavic countries in Central Europe continues nowadays and is marked and reinforced by language differences and language practices that maintain group boundaries and status hierarchies during ordinary cross‐national encounters (Brown & Haeger, 1999; Petrjánošová & Graf, 2012; see Discussion for more on this topic).
Against this backdrop of historical tensions, several forms of contact now thrive among the citizens of these five countries—including student exchanges, shopping trips, tourism and work experiences. In this context, individuals of different group memberships meet regularly and frequently. We expected these ordinary, unstructured, everyday encounters to vary amply and freely in valence (and to do so more than in polarized settings), thus allowing for an incisive test of our predictions.
With our focus on varied, daily and unstructured contact experiences, this research offers some discontinuity with the traditional intergroup contact literature's focus on positive and sanctioned experiences in conflict‐ridden settings (Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Nonetheless, our approach is desirable because it can extend the scope of intergroup contact theory beyond its traditional research paradigm and test its applicability to more ordinary settings of intergroup contact. Many contemporary multi‐ethnic Western societies are characterized by prolonged periods of formal peace that involve undercurrents of intergroup pressure. In these societies, people typically refrain from openly expressing intergroup animosity, let alone reacting with aggression (Pettigrew & Meertens, 1995); yet, this does not equate to wholly harmonious intergroup relations or to intergroup relationships that are inconsequential for individuals or groups. Therefore, while we see the merit of the intergroup contact literature being quintessentially a literature of corrective interventions for conflict‐ridden contexts, we regard more benign settings and settings with more covert forms of intergroup tensions also worthy of examination through the lens of intergroup contact theory (see also Pettigrew & Tropp, 2011, Chapter 12). In addition, to extend the scope of traditional contact research, the composite multi‐national nature of our sample from Central Europe will enable us to test the generality of our findings and strengthen the basis of our conclusions.
METHOD
Participants
A sample of 1276 university students from five Central European countries was recruited via the university email systems (Austria n = 146; the Czech Republic n = 69111
The reason for the higher number of Czech participants was that the research originated in the Czech Republic and the data sampling was supported by the Czech Science Foundation. Hence, the research focused on the situation in Central Europe from the point of view of the Czech Republic. Furthermore, we were interested in mutual perceptions of two groups that meet in any given border region. As such, we sampled participants from four neighboring countries together with Czech participants coming from four different border regions, resulting in a larger Czech subsample.
; Germany n = 132; Poland n = 134; and Slovakia n = 173; Mage = 23.98, SD = 5.64; 78% women).
Procedure and Materials
A link to an online questionnaire was emailed or posted on a webpage of cooperating universities. Participation was anonymous, and participants were free to decide whether and when to participate. The study questionnaire was translated into four languages so that all participants filled out materials in their native language.
Participants first provided their demographic details. They were then asked to recall and describe their contact experiences with people from the neighboring country following these instructions: “Can you recall any experiences you had with a [outgroup person; nationality label provided; e.g., “a Czech man or woman”] during your visit abroad or here in home country [country label provided; e.g., “Poland”]? How did the outgroup member [nationality label provided] behave in that particular situation? How did you behave? Please describe the situation, below.” 22
The instructions mentioned both person and situation not to skew responses towards one mode of framing over another.
Respondents were invited to describe their intergroup contact experience in a large type‐in text box. Depending on the specific border region that they lived in, different Czech participants described contact with different target outgroups (with Austrians: n = 165; Germans: n = 176; Poles: n = 174; or Slovaks: n = 176); participants from the other four Central European countries were always asked to describe contact with Czechs.
After several filler items (see Hřebíčková & Graf, 2014), a five‐item measure of national identification followed using a 5‐point Likert‐type scale (Leach et al., 2008). At the end of the questionnaire, participants indicated their attitudes towards the target outgroup in two ways. First, they reported the warmth of their feelings towards the outgroup on a feeling thermometer (Haddock, Zanna, & Esses, 1993); the thermometer used 30‐point increments with anchors cold and warm. Second, participants completed a semantic differential item using a 5‐point Likert‐type scale to indicate how good or bad they perceived the target outgroup. The two measures were coded so that higher values indicated warmer feelings or more positive attitudes towards the outgroup. The correlation between the two measures was moderate, r(1276) = .41, p < .001.
A content analysis (Neuendorf, 2002) was carried out to analyze participants' descriptions of intergroup contact. We prepared a codebook with categories relevant to the focus of this study: positive and negative contact valence pertaining to the person and the situation. Descriptions that referred directly to the contact partners or their behavior were coded as person framing (e.g., “my classmate worked on the project with great effort”); descriptions that referred to the context of contact were coded as situation framing (e.g., “the atmosphere in the restaurant was quite tensed”). Explicit positive references to valence were coded along the positivity category as positive (e.g., “the family I stayed with was doing its best to prepare an entertaining program for me”); explicit negative references to valence were coded along the negativity dimension as negative (e.g., “this whole experience has knocked them off the pedestal of our paragons”). The combination of valence coding (positive and negative) and framing coding (person and situation) resulted in four categories along which all and each of the open statements were categorized: (i) person positivity (present/absent); (ii) person negativity (present/absent); (iii) situation positivity (present/absent); and (iv) situation negativity (present/absent).
Through this coding method, open accounts of cross‐border contact could be coded as containing both positive and negative valence of the person and/or the situation at the same time. For example, in a rich account on romantic contact with her Czech boyfriend, an Austrian participant mentioned both her boyfriend's positive and negative attributes (e.g., being smart and enterprising vs. too comfy and not searching for a job hard enough); hence, this self‐reported account was coded as including reference to “person positivity” and reference to “person negativity.” In her detailed description, this participant also appraised the positive and negative contextual elements of their contact (e.g., stability of the relationship vs. difficulties in acculturation in a foreign country); therefore, this self‐reported account was also coded for reference to “situation positivity” and “situation negativity.” In other words, in this particularly rich open account of a specific contact experience, all four categories were coded as “present”. More often, the coded material included reference to a smaller subset of categories and was therefore coded as “absent” on other categories. Thus, in all cases, each individual report of contact generated four codes. On the other hand, the coding did not take into account the number of positive and negative experiences that a participant had mentioned at the same time.
Data from the two sides of each border region were pooled and always analyzed by two independent coders. Overall, we employed five coders of different nationalities who were fluent in the languages spoken on both sides of a particular border. The dyads always consisted of coders of different nationalities to control for possible ethnocentric biases influencing coding decisions. The inter‐rater reliability analysis indicated good agreement between coders beyond chance for all of the coded categories in all eight border regions (all Cohen's Kappas ≥ .72, Mdn = .81; Banerjee, Capozzoli, McSweeney, & Sinha, 1999).
We treated contact valence uni‐dimensionally and bi‐dimensionally. For the uni‐dimensional approach, we compared and contrasted positive and negative contact descriptions (coding: 0 = positive contact, 1 = negative contact) separately for the situation and person framing. This approach resulted in two vectors (situation valence: 0–1; person valence: 0–1). We call this approach uni‐dimensional because, in this, positive contact and negative contact represent two opposing poles of a single evaluative dimension. In the bi‐dimensional approach, we coded for the presence (coded as 1) as well as the absence (coded as 0) of both positive and negative contact, again separately for situation and person framing. Hence, this second approach resulted in four vectors (i.e., person positivity: 0–1; person negativity: 0–1; situation positivity: 0–1; and situation negativity: 0–1). We call this second approach bi‐dimensional because, in this, positivity and negativity are two independent dimensions.33
Positivity and negativity were not significantly correlated in either person framing, r(1276) = −.02, p = .42, or situation‐framing, r(1276) = −.03, p = .22, thus justifying the use of a bi‐dimensional approach.
RESULTS
Frequency of Positive versus Negative Contact
The frequency of positive and negative accounts of contact experiences was first inspected within the uni‐dimensional approach (Table 1). In naturalistic settings, we expected positive contact to outnumber negative contact.44
As a check on the psychological significance of the intergroup settings under consideration, we compared participants' ingroup and outgroup attitudes. Our data showed a clear presence of ingroup favoritism in four of the five countries (the Czech republic: t(690) = 15.09, p ˂ .001; Germany: t(131) = 6.98, p ˂ .001; Austria: t(145) = 2.44, p = .02; Poland: t(133) = 0.12, n.s.; Slovakia: t(171) = 7.14, p ˂ .001).
In line with our prediction, accounts of positive experiences were four times more prevalent than accounts of negative experiences in both person framing, χ2(673) = 215.69, p < .001, and situation framing, χ2(576) = 100.00, p < .001. The greater prevalence of positive experiences held also within the bi‐dimensional coding: Contact positivity was twice as prevalent as contact negativity. The bi‐dimensional coding also revealed that a large proportion of participants did not describe their past contact experience in an evaluative way (see “absent” rows in the bottom part of Table 1).This large representation of non‐evaluative contact accounts suggests that self‐selection biases favoring the recruitment of individuals with relatively polarized experiences from intergroup contact may have had limited impact on our data. Interestingly, irrespective of coding approach, the distribution of positive and negative experiences was virtually identical in both situation‐ and person‐framed contact descriptions. Confidence intervals confirmed that positivity and negativity frequencies differed significantly in both framings: person positivity 95% CIs [0.50, 0.56]; person negativity [0.21, 0.25]; situation positivity [0.36, 0.42]; situation negativity [0.21, 0.25].55
In order to control for the possible influence of social desirability on the reporting of positive versus negative contact experiences, we checked for the moderating influence of participants' tendency to self‐characterize themselves as high versus low on socially desirable self‐attributes: neuroticism, agreeableness and conscientiousness in the Big Five Markers Inventory (Hřebíčková et al., 2002). Previous research indicates that these personality dimensions are sensitive and implicit markers of individual differences in desirable responding (Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1989, 1992; Pauls & Stemmler, 2003; Stöber, 2001). Hence, after a median split of the three personality dimensions, we examined the distribution of positive and negative contact experiences. This ancillary analysis showed that the distribution of positive and negative experience did not differ depending on participants' self‐rated neuroticism, agreeableness or conscientiousness (ps ≥ .10). The only difference in distribution was found for situation negativity, χ2(1276) = 3.78, p = .05: Participants who rated themselves high on conscientiousness listed slightly less negative evaluations of the contact situation than expected and the opposite was true for participants who rated themselves low on conscientiousness. This difference however disappeared when correcting the p‐value for repeated tests. Altogether, these ancillary results assist us in ruling out social desirability influences on our prevalence findings.
The comparison of frequencies between the five countries confirmed similar distributions of positive and negative contact experiences as for the overall sample.66
We found limited evidence of cross‐sample differences in contact experiences. Within the uni‐dimensional coding, the only significant differences were between Czechs and Poles in positive person valence (73%, 95% CIs [.68, .78] vs. 90%, [.83, .97], respectively; 78% in the overall sample) and between Czechs and Slovaks in positive situation valence (66%, 95% CIs [.59, .73] vs. 85%, [.77, .93], respectively; 71% in the overall sample); for all other 18 comparisons within the uni‐dimensional coding, ps > .05. Within the bi‐dimensional coding, the only significant differences were found between Germans and Slovaks on person positivity (69%, 95% CIs [.59, .79] vs. 43%, [.32, .54], respectively; 53% in the overall sample) and Czechs and Slovaks on situation positivity (36%, 95% CIs [.30, .42] vs. 54%, [.44, .64], respectively; 39% in the overall sample); for all other 38 comparisons within the bi‐dimensional coding, ps > .05. Hence, the basic pattern of greater prevalence for positive vs. negative contact accounts held substantially invariant across the five national groups.
n | % | M (SD) Outgroup attitudes | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Feeling thermometer | Bad–good item | ||||
Uni‐dimensional coding: | |||||
1. Person valence | – Positive | 527 | 78 | 18.33 (6.15) | 3.67 (0.69) |
– Negative | 146 | 22 | 14.04 (5.88) | 3.25 (0.71) | |
2. Situation valence | – Positive | 408 | 71 | 18.76 (5.94) | 3.69 (0.67) |
– Negative | 168 | 29 | 15.29 (6.59) | 3.36 (0.76) | |
Bi‐dimensional coding: | |||||
1. Person positivity | – Present | 681 | 53 | 17.88 (6.21) | 3.63 (0.70) |
– Absent | 595 | 47 | 17.49 (6.22) | 3.51 (0.75) | |
2. Person negativity | – Present | 300 | 23 | 15.22 (6.13) | 3.38 (0.72) |
– Absent | 976 | 77 | 18.46 (6.04) | 3.64 (0.72) | |
3. Situation positivity | – Present | 503 | 39 | 18.50 (6.07) | 3.67 (0.68) |
– Absent | 773 | 61 | 17.18 (6.26) | 3.51 (0.75) | |
4. Situation negativity | – Present | 263 | 21 | 16.05 (6.62) | 3.43 (0.74) |
– Absent | 1013 | 79 | 18.13 (6.03) | 3.61 (0.72) |
- Note: Outgroup attitudes on the feeling thermometer were measured with a 30‐point scale and on bad‐good item with a 5‐point scale. Higher values indicate more positive attitudes.
Effects of Person‐ versus Situation‐Framed Valence on Outgroup Attitudes
To assess the effects of person‐ and situation‐framed valence in predicting outgroup attitudes, we employed a series of linear regression analyses. When predicting outgroup attitudes from person valence alone (see top of Table 2), negative experiences with outgroup members, as compared with positive ones, significantly predicted worse attitudes on both the feeling thermometer and the bad–good item.77
In simple linear regressions with a dummy‐coded categorical predictor, the group means are derived in the following way: The mean of the outcome variable for the 0‐coded group (positive contact in our uni‐dimensional approach) equals to the constant or intercept; as such, the 0‐coded group acts as the benchmark comparison group. The b‐value of the 1‐coded group is added to or subtracted from the constant (depending on the sign of b‐value) to compute the mean for the 1‐coded group (negative contact in our uni‐dimensional coding).
We obtained the same pattern when predicting outgroup attitudes from situation valence alone (see middle of Table 2): Contact marked by a negative context predicted significantly worse outgroup attitudes as compared with contact marked by a positive context. These findings indicate that when contrasting only positive with negative experiences, contact valence affected outgroup attitudes in the predicted direction in both person and situation framings.
Feeling thermometer | Bad–good item | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | β | b | SE | β | |
Constant | 18.33 | 0.27 | 3.67 | 0.03 | ||
Person valence | −4.29 | 0.57 | −.28******
p < .001. |
−0.43 | 0.07 | −.24******
p < .001. |
F | 56.68******
p < .001. |
42.62******
p < .001. |
||||
R2 | .08 | .06 | ||||
b | SE | β | b | SE | β | |
Constant | 18.76 | 0.30 | 3.69 | 0.04 | ||
Situation valence | −3.47 | 0.56 | −.25******
p < .001. |
−0.34 | 0.06 | −.21******
p < .001. |
F | 38.12******
p < .001. |
27.50******
p < .001. |
||||
R2 | .06 | .05 | ||||
b | SE | β | b | SE | β | |
Constant | 18.85 | 0.42 | 3.79 | 0.05 | ||
Person valence | −3.22 | 1.03 | −.21****
p < .01, |
−0.39 | 0.11 | −.23******
p < .001. |
Situation valence | −1.57 | 0.97 | −.11 | −0.20 | 0.11 | −.13 |
F | 15.13******
p < .001. |
18.36******
p < .001. |
||||
R2 | .09 | .10 |
- Note: In both person‐ and situation‐framed valence, positive contact was dummy coded as 0 and negative contact as 1. Outgroup attitudes on feeling thermometer were measured with a 30‐point scale and on bad–good item with a 5‐point scale. Higher values indicate more positive attitudes.
- * p < .05,
- ** p < .01,
- *** p < .001.
Importantly, when entering both person‐ and situation‐framed valence into the regression equation simultaneously (see bottom of Table 2), situation valence no longer uniquely predicted outgroup attitudes. This finding shows that when considering only positive and negative contact experiences, person‐framed valence was a more robust predictor of outgroup attitudes than situation‐framed valence. Participants' nationality did not influence any of these effects,88
We conducted a multi‐group analysis in order to test the invariance of regression parameters across the five different countries. Using AMOS, we fixed the values of the standardized regression coefficients between nations. For both outcome variables, the fit indices of the model provided evidence for invariance across different countries (feeling thermometer: χ2 = 35.96, df = 20, p = .016, χ2/df = 1.798, RMSEA = 0.050, pclose = 0.456, Hoelter .05 = 279; bad–good item: χ2 = 28.74, df = 20, p = .093, χ2/df = 1.437, RMSEA = 0.037, pclose = 0.740, Hoelter .05 = 348).
and participants' social desirability concerns did not affect them appreciably either,99
The coefficients associated with the two uni‐dimensional valence predictors remained substantially unchanged when controlling for the three proxies of social desirability—self‐rated neuroticism, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Furthermore, none of the three proxies moderated the findings for association between contact valence framing indices and outgroup attitudes. The only isolated exception was a weak moderation by conscientiousness for the link between person valence and outgroup attitudes on the feeling thermometer, ΔR2 = .01, ΔF(1, 667) = 4.92, p = .03, B = 1.53, t(667) = 2.22. The Johnson–Neyman technique indicated that the negative effect of (negative) person valence (dummy coded with 0 for positive and 1 for negative) on outgroup feeling thermometer was stronger among those lower on self‐conscientiousness than among those higher on self‐conscientiousness.
indicating that the pattern of results held constant across all five participants' national groups and variations in an indirect proxy of social desirability. Furthermore, controlling for identification with one's nationality (satisfaction and centrality; Leach et al., 2008) did not change the pattern of results in either of the regression models.
Overall, these findings confirm our expectations that valence appraisals centered around particular contact partners (person‐framed valence) are more powerful predictors of outgroup attitudes than valence appraisals centered around the contact situation.
Effects of Negative versus Positive Contact Moderated by Person and Situation Framing
We first performed two multiple linear regression analyses separately for person and situation framings to compare the effects of the presence of contact positivity and of contact negativity to their absence. When entering person positivity and negativity simultaneously into the model, we found that reference to person negativity in participants' contact descriptions predicted significantly worsened attitudes (as compared with contact descriptions where person negativity was missing) on both the feeling thermometer and the bad–good item. Reference to person positivity, on the other hand, was not a significant predictor of outgroup attitudes on the feeling thermometer and was a visibly weaker predictor on the bad–good item (see top of Table 3). These findings indicate that while contact descriptions where person negativity was present associated with worse outgroup attitudes than descriptions where person negativity was absent, the presence of person positivity did not make such a difference to outgroup attitudes as compared with its absence. Hence, consistent with our hypothesis, on the person‐framing variables, we found clear evidence of valence asymmetry: That is, person negativity was more influential in shaping outgroup attitudes than person positivity. In contrast to person framing, we found no evidence for valence asymmetry in the situation‐framed variables. When replicating the regression analyses with the situation‐framed variables, we found that the effect of situation negativity and positivity on outgroup attitudes was comparable in magnitude on both the feeling thermometer and the bad–good item (see middle of Table 3).
Feeling thermometer | Bad–good item | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
b | SE | β | b | SE | β | |
Constant | 18.29 | 0.27 | 3.57 | 0.03 | ||
Person negativity | −3.23 | 0.40 | −.22******
p < .001. |
−0.25 | 0.05 | −.15******
p < .001. |
Person positivity | 0.33 | 0.34 | .03 | 0.12 | 0.04 | .08****
p < .01, |
F | 33.19******
p < .001. |
19.20******
p < .001. |
||||
R2 | .05 | .03 | ||||
b | SE | β | b | SE | β | |
Constant | 17.62 | 0.24 | 3.55 | 0.03 | ||
Situation negativity | −2.03 | 0.42 | −.13******
p < .001. |
−0.17 | 0.05 | −.10******
p < .001. |
Situation positivity | 1.26 | 0.35 | .10******
p < .001. |
0.15 | 0.04 | .10******
p < .001. |
F | 18.52******
p < .001. |
13.20******
p < .001. |
||||
R2 | .03 | .02 | ||||
b | SE | β | b | SE | β | |
Constant | 18.14 | 0.32 | 3.54 | 0.04 | ||
Person negativity | −2.75 | 0.42 | −.19******
p < .001. |
−0.21 | 0.05 | −.12******
p < .001. |
Person positivity | 0.17 | 0.34 | .01 | 0.11 | 0.04 | .07****
p < .01, |
Situation negativity | −1.19 | 0.44 | −.08****
p < .01, |
−0.09 | 0.05 | −.05 |
Situation positivity | 0.93 | 0.35 | .07****
p < .01, |
0.12 | 0.04 | .08****
p < .01, |
F | 20.27******
p < .001. |
12.65******
p < .001. |
||||
R2 | .06 | .04 |
- Note: The presence of negative or positive contact was dummy coded as 1 and the absence of negative or positive contact as 0. Outgroup attitudes on the feeling thermometer were measured with a 30‐point scale and on bad–good item with a 5‐point scale. Higher values indicate more positive attitudes.
- * p < .05,
- ** p < .01,
- *** p < .001.
To examine the unique contribution to attitudes of positive and negative contact that is framed around the contact person and the contact situation, we entered all four contact valence predictors into one model simultaneously (see bottom of Table 3). In line with our expectations, on both outcome variables, person negativity was found to be the strongest unique predictor of outgroup attitudes. That is, person negativity was the most influential predictor of outgroup attitudes; it was more influential than person positivity, situation negativity and situation positivity. These results were again unaffected by social desirability concerns, participants' nationality and national identification.1010
When controlling for the three social desirability proxies, the coefficients associated with the four bi‐dimensional valence predictors remained substantially unchanged. Furthermore, multi‐group analysis testing the invariance of regression parameters across the five different countries again indicated generalizability of our findings in both models with different measures of outgroup attitudes (feeling thermometer: χ2 = 76.30, df = 36, p = .001, χ2/df = 2.121, RMSEA = 0.030, pclose = 1.0, Hoelter .05 = 853; bad–good item: χ2 = 52.31, df = 36, p = .039, χ2/df = 1.453, RMSEA = 0.019, pclose = 1.0, Hoelter .05 = 1243).
Importantly, as we had anticipated, the previously neglected situation valence also exerted some unique influence on outgroup attitudes; however, valence appraisals framed around the contact person were comparatively more influential, especially when negative in nature. Hence, valence asymmetries on outgroup attitudes were moderated by the framing of the contact experience, and person negativity was the most robust predictor of outgroup attitudes.
DISCUSSION
The present research started from the premise that the frequency and effect of positive contact and negative contact must be considered simultaneously for a fuller understanding of intergroup contact's impact on outgroup attitudes. In line with our expectations, we found that while negative intergroup contact was relatively more influential than positive intergroup contact in shaping outgroup attitudes, the frequency of positive contact experiences unquestionably outnumbered negative contact's frequency. This pattern of results held invariant across respondents from five Central European nations, who had been surveyed with a non‐obtrusive tool for their ordinary and non‐structured experiences of cross‐border contact. Hence, we found some evidence that in real‐world settings, the disproportionately stronger influence of negative contact may be significantly attenuated by the disproportionate larger frequency of the less influential positive contact.
Combining the relative frequency and influence of positive and negative contact in one study design enabled us to reconcile contradictory outlooks of intergroup contact. In light of our findings, we believe it is legitimate to infer that past studies subsumed in Pettigrew and Tropp's (2006) meta‐analysis captured the relative greater prevalence of positive contact in naturalistic settings and its modest effects on outgroup attitudes. We corroborated findings of this line of research by showing that the beneficial effects of positive contact are relatively widespread, although not as strong in size as the effects of negative contact. At the same time, we validated the findings of research by Barlow, Paolini and colleagues (Barlow et al., 2012; Paolini et al., 2010; Paolini, Harwood et al., 2014) by demonstrating that negative contact is more consequential for intergroup relations also outside the psychology laboratory, in everyday intergroup encounters in real‐world settings. In the following sections, we discuss the wider implications of our findings for theory and interventions in more detail.
Ecological Evidence for the Prevalence of Positive Contact
Reports of positive contact experiences in our data markedly outweighed reports of negative contact experiences. When contrasting only positive and negative experiences, positive contact was four times more frequent than negative contact. When extending our focus to include also non‐evaluative accounts of contact, positive contact was still twice as frequent as negative contact. Because this pattern held across different coding approaches (uni‐dimensional/bi‐dimensional), contact framings (person/situation) and nationalities (Austria, the Czech Republic, Germany, Poland and Slovakia), our data offer a solid basis to conclude that positive contact experiences may be disproportionately represented in many peaceful real social settings.
While our results are in line with earlier reports of relative prevalence of positive contact (Aberson & Gaffney, 2009; Barlow et al., 2012; Dhont & Van Hiel, 2009; Pettigrew, 2008), it is key to appreciate that they surpass them all in ecological validity because of the wider range of experiences our unobtrusive measurement tool sampled. For example, a study by Pettigrew (2008), examining autochthon contact with immigrants in Germany, found that more than two thirds of their participants reported having had interesting conversations with or being helped by a foreigner; however, only one third reported being pestered by a foreigner. Aberson and Gaffney (2009) found a similar pattern of results with a slightly wider range of positivity and negativity behavioral markers (e.g., inquiring about close, equal, intimate contact vs. insulting, harassing, ridiculing or intimidating). Yet, participants' reporting was still constrained to several but a finite number of behaviors, which may be relatively extreme, unrepresentative or uncommon in everyday encounters. Our prevalence estimates, by being drawn from a free‐recall method, were instead completely unconstrained and free to sample from all types of experiences and behavioral markers.
Our approach is also superior to previous measures surveying participants' frequency of positive and negative experiences with broad but still direct closed‐ended questions (e.g., “On average, how frequently do you have positive/good vs. negative/bad contact with the outgroup?”; Study 2 of Barlow et al., 2012; Study 2 of Dhont & Van Hiel, 2009). Critically, the method we used was free from valence probes and allowed us to sample also non‐evaluative appraisals of past contact, thus extending further the breadth and ecological validity of our analysis of contact experiences. Ancillary analyses confirmed that our results were unaffected by social desirability concerns—at least as assessed by our indirect proxies drawn from the Big 5—and, thus, were more likely than previous results, to reflect unbiased estimates of contact valence. However, future research should employ more direct and validated measures of social desirability to corroborate our findings.
While the multi‐sample/multi‐setting nature of our data increases the confidence in the generalizability of our prevalence findings, a few important caveats are in order. First, we are far from arguing that positive contact pervades all intergroup settings. Rather, we expect this prevalence of positive contact to reasonably extend to similar, relatively peaceful and non‐segregated contexts that offer—like the settings we investigated here—plenty of opportunities for face‐to‐face, unstructured, daily exchanges. Conflict‐ridden settings, obviously, continue to exist; here, the detrimental effects of negative contact, instead of being attenuated by the greater prevalence of positive contact, are most likely further compounded by the relatively higher prevalence of negative contact (e.g., Dhont, Cornelis, & Van Hiel, 2010). Prevalence findings are also likely moderated by type of contact. For example, negative experiences with outgroups might again outnumber positive experiences in parasocial or televised contact (i.e., news about outgroup members disseminated through media) or in socially mediated or indirect forms of intergroup contact (e.g., gossips). Hence, both the type of contact and the ecology of particular intergroup relations need careful consideration.
New Evidence for Negative Contact's Stronger Influence on Outgroup Attitudes
When comparing strength of effects, we found that negative contact was a better predictor of outgroup attitudes than positive contact (see also Barlow et al., 2012; Dhont & Van Hiel, 2009). This valence asymmetry, as anticipated, was particularly pronounced in contact experiences framed around the contact person, rather than the contact situation.
This pattern of findings for contact prevalence is different from some of the extant data but in a meaningful way. We have discussed already our methodological objections to Pettigrew's (2008) operationalization of contact valence; these objections obviously extend to his results for the contact valence–attitude link. More interestingly, Stark et al. (2013) recently reported coefficients of equivalent magnitude for the longitudinal effects of positive and negative attitudes towards classmates of different ethnicities on the attitudes towards the ethnic groups. In institutionalized settings, because contact is carefully structured and monitored by sanctioning authorities, it is not surprising that negative contact does not reach the strength and connotations of negative contact as it is or can be experienced in unstructured and uncontrolled settings (see similar findings in a meta‐analysis of laboratory data; Paolini, McIntyre et al., 2014). Independent data by Bekhuis, Ruiter, and Coenders (2013) support this interpretation. In this study, Dutch youngsters' experiences with minority individuals were once again surveyed, however, this time, in several social settings that varied in degree of structuring, monitoring and sanctioning by authorities (e.g., the classroom vs. the neighborhood). As in Stark et al., Bekhuis and colleagues found that positive and negative contact had equal effects on ethnic distance in the highly structured, monitored and sanctioned context of the classroom; in line with our findings, they found that negative (vs. positive) contact was more influential in the unstructured and unregulated neighborhood setting.
Overall, these meaningful variations in the relative prominence of positive versus negative experiences with the outgroup as a function of key contact features—e.g., structured–unstructured and intimate–casual—further emphasize the need to pay close attention to the ecology of different types of intergroup encounters.
Novel Evidence of Person‐Based and Situation‐Based Generalizations
A secondary goal of our research was to highlight and spell out two distinct sources of influence on outgroup attitudes: The valence framed around contact situation and contact person. While previous intergroup research has focused on the characteristics of the outgroup interaction partners (Brown & Hewstone, 2005; Hewstone & Brown, 1986; Stark et al., 2013), in our study, we advanced the possibility and found some preliminary evidence that situation‐framed valence uniquely contributes to outgroup attitudes during intergroup encounters. While person‐framed (vs. situation‐framed) valence proved to be a stronger predictor of outgroup attitudes when we contrasted exclusively positive with exclusively negative experiences, situation valence predicted outgroup attitudes over and above the effect of person valence when we included non‐evaluative contact descriptions in the mix. This means that more precise predictions about the effects of intergroup contact can be achieved by taking into account the characteristics and behaviors of the contact partners and the characteristics and appraisals of the contact context more broadly.
Interestingly, the person versus situation framing significantly moderated the valence asymmetries in prominence we discussed earlier. These asymmetries were less pronounced or absent altogether when intergroup contact was framed around the contact situation; as we had anticipated, they were most pronounced when valence appraisals were framed around the characteristics and behaviors of the contact partner. These findings are important as they suggest that it is not negativity per se to exert superior influence on attitudes (for an extensive discussion of valence asymmetries in other domains, see Baumeister et al., 2001) and to skew intergroup relations towards negativity. Rather, it is a specific type of negativity that is most dangerous and unduly detrimental for intergroup relations. It is the negativity that can be experienced as inherently and more stably associated with the outgroup that we need to fear the most (Hewstone, 1989; Pettigrew, 1979). Wilder and colleagues (1996) demonstrated the beneficial effects of positive stable and dispositional qualities of contact partners. We believe we unraveled the negative flip side of this phenomenon.
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
Neither surveys nor field studies with cross‐sectional designs can ascertain the direction of causality when considering the relation between intergroup contact and outgroup attitudes. Hence, as much as our participants' negative (vs. positive) contact experiences may have caused the worsening (vs. improving) of their outgroup attitudes, we cannot rule out the possibility that their outgroup attitudes caused them to selectively retrieve negative versus positive past experiences with the outgroup. However, other studies have brought evidence for person‐to‐group transfers without group‐to‐person transfers (see, e.g., Stark et al., 2013, for longitudinal evidence).
We employed open reports of intergroup contact experiences in order to avoid biased frequency estimates of participants' positive versus negative past experiences with outgroup members (for extensive discussions of common biases in frequency estimates, see Schwarz, 1999, 2007). Using these open reports, we expected participants to readily and accurately retrieve their most salient and typical experience with outgroup members (see, e.g., Rothbart, Sriram, & Davis‐Stitt, 1996, for consistent evidence), thus allowing us to compare the incidence of positive versus negative experiences between‐participants within the whole sample in a relatively unbiased manner. Yet, open‐ended reports of past experiences are subjected to their own memory and communication biases (see Schwarz, 2007). Hence, while the methodology employed in our study represents a novel approach, it is not intended to replace but rather complement traditional frequency estimates. Future studies should verify the prevalence of positive (vs negative) experiences with alternative and possibly more accurate frequency estimates, such as those drawn from observational and diary methods (see, e.g., Page‐Gould, 2012; however, cf. Schwarz, 2007).
Additional limitations of our research were that our participant sample comprised university students and our intergroup setting was relatively benign. Because we wanted to explore a rather underresearched area in the intergroup contact literature, we chose to start our investigation with a convenience sample. Furthermore, our sample contained a higher number of nationals from one country, although participants' nationality did not moderate any of the reported effects. Hence, our findings need to be further validated in different intergroup settings with more representative samples that allow for broader generalization.
Notwithstanding these limitations, our research has the merit of having unveiled a new source of influence on outgroup attitudes— situation‐framed valence. Yet, the proportion of total variance in outgroup attitudes that we were able to explain was, in absolute terms, still small. Even when including all our four contact valence indices, only one tenth of the total variance in outgroup attitudes was accounted for. It is critical to draw attention to the fact that this low predictive power is not unique to our investigation; in fact, the amount of explained variance in outgroup attitudes we were able to isolate already surpasses the average amount (approximately 5%) explained by the average intergroup contact study, at least as included in Pettigrew and Tropp's (2006) extensive meta‐analysis.
This large gulf of unexplained variance equates to a big challenge ahead of the intergroup contact researchers as they try to improve the precision of their predictions and the power of their explanations of the processes that take place during intergroup contact experiences. The present work points towards the fruitfulness of a simultaneous assessment of the frequency and the effects of positive and negative experiences, in different types of intergroup contact and in different types of intergroup settings. This broader stance is likely to offer a more sophisticated and fuller understanding of the conditions that enhance and that inhibit the effects of contact on outgroup attitudes.
A broader challenge for scholars and practitioners is the translation of intergroup contact findings into real‐world interventions that improve intergroup relations and reduce social inequality. Although intergroup contact has beneficial effects on outgroup attitudes, some scholars have recognized its implication in sustaining social inequalities (Dixon & Levine, 2012; Saguy & Chernyak‐Hai, 2012; Saguy, Tausch, Dovidio, & Pratto, 2009). Namely, better outgroup attitudes of disadvantaged minority groups achieved through more intergroup contact with privileged groups come—at times—with minority individuals' inability to recognize and challenge the injustice that their group suffers. Under these circumstances, intergroup contact sustains paternalistic relations. Besides highlighting the more obvious detrimental effects of recalling negative contact experiences for outgroup attitudes, the textual data included in this article shed additional light on the less obvious detrimental effects of intergroup contact in the social context under investigation.
Language is an important marker of cross‐national inequalities and status hierarchies in Central Europe. German is the language of wealthier and high status countries (i.e., Germany and Austria; see Brown & Haeger, 1999), and it is typically used in contact between people from Germanic and Slavic countries, independent of the place of encounter. Although people from the less affluent (i.e., Slavic) countries usually recognize their discrimination based on grounds of language use, another study in Central European region found that respondents who had positive contact with the outgroup tended to downplay the challenges posed by the use of the higher status group's native language (Petrjánošová & Graf, 2012). For example, Czech participants, who reported positive experiences from intergroup contact with Germans or Austrians, were also those who recalled paternalistic communication patterns (e.g., praising Germans or Austrians for speaking German slowly with them or for not using their dialect but a standard form of German during contact with Czechs even in the Czech Republic). While the social consequences of this inability to recognize and react against language‐based discrimination in Central Europe might be less profound and far‐reaching than in more closed and stratified systems, we suspect that these dynamics still operate to obstruct progress towards more egalitarian intergroup relations. Future research on the understudied topic of negative intergroup contact should go beyond its detrimental effects on outgroup attitudes and investigate its role in identifying and challenging social injustice. Because, as Dixon, Levine, Reicher, and Durrheim (2012) aptly remarked, getting people to like one another more might not be the solution to tackle discrimination but rather a way to obstructing progress towards social justice in a fuller sense.
Concluding Remarks
We started this research endeavor with Baumeister and colleagues' (2001) conjecture that “good may prevail over bad by superior force of numbers” (p. 323); we returned empirical evidence that this may be more than a mere conjecture in intergroup settings. When assessing the ordinary and unstructured intergroup contact experiences of the people from five Central European countries, we found that negative experiences of contact, while more influential on intergroup attitudes than positive experiences, were relatively infrequent and uncommon in people's accounts of ordinary life accounts. On the contrary, the relatively less influential positive contact was a very common and widespread experience for our participants. This means that the greater prevalence of positive contact can possibly compensate for the greater prominence of negative contact and ultimately translate in modest but relatively stable net improvements in outgroup attitudes after contact.
These findings invite to a fuller understanding of intergroup contact effects through the simultaneous consideration of both relative frequency and relative strength of associations between positive and negative contact and outgroup attitudes. As such, not only they bridge and integrate together two distinct and prima face incompatible outlooks on the effect of positive and negative contact on outgroup attitudes (cf. Paolini et al., 2010; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), we believe they contribute to bring intergroup contact theory closer to the complexities and the ecology of the social reality that it tries to explain.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by a grant 13‐25656S from the Czech Science Foundation awarded to the first author, by RVO: 68081740 from the Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic and an Australian Research Council grant awarded to the second and third authors (DP0770704). The manuscript was prepared during a research visit by Sylvie Graf to the University of Newcastle, Australia, sponsored by an Award for International Collaboration from the Czech Academy of Sciences and by the University of Newcastle's Emerging Research Leadership Program awarded to Stefania Paolini and Mark Rubin.
REFERENCES
Citing Literature
Number of times cited according to CrossRef: 88
- Rhiannon N. Turner, Gordon Hodson, Kristof Dhont, The role of individual differences in understanding and enhancing intergroup contact, Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10.1111/spc3.12533, 14, 6, (2020).
- Francesca Prati, Silvia Moscatelli, Miles Hewstone, Monica Rubini, The effects of recalling positive and negative contacts on linguistic discrimination towards migrant people, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103970, 89, (103970), (2020).
- Giulia Fuochi, Alberto Voci, Jessica Boin, Miles Hewstone, Close to me: The importance of closeness versus superficiality in explaining the positive‐negative contact asymmetry, European Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1002/ejsp.2667, 50, 4, (766-782), (2020).
- Fei Huang, Kuankuan Shi, Mingjie Zhou, Sofia Stathi, Loris Vezzali, Can interethnic contact between majority (Han) and minority (Uyghur) people in China influence sense of Chinese national Community? The role of positive and negative direct, extended and vicarious intergroup contact, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2020.05.008, 77, (125-139), (2020).
- Louis Talay, David De Coninck, Exploring the link between personality traits and European attitudes towards refugees, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2020.04.002, 77, (13-24), (2020).
- Max Reinwald, Florian Kunze, Being Different, Being Absent? A Dynamic Perspective on Demographic Dissimilarity and Absenteeism in Blue-Collar Teams, Academy of Management Journal, 10.5465/amj.2018.0290, 63, 3, (660-684), (2020).
- Tina F. Keil, Miriam Koschate, Variations in subjective definitions of everyday situations as intergroup contact, British Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1111/bjso.12372, 59, 4, (965-991), (2020).
- David J. Sparkman, Katarzyna Hamer, Seeing the human in everyone: Multicultural experiences predict more positive intergroup attitudes and humanitarian helping through identification with all humanity, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2020.08.007, 79, (121-134), (2020).
- Rebecca Loader, Joanne Hughes, Andrea Furey, ‘By law, custom or local atmosphere’: Exploring institutional support in school‐based contact programmes, British Educational Research Journal, 10.1002/berj.3612, 0, 0, (2020).
- Tina F. Keil, Miriam Koschate, Mark Levine, Contact Logger: Measuring everyday intergroup contact experiences in near-time, Behavior Research Methods, 10.3758/s13428-019-01335-w, (2020).
- Pascal Schlechter, Jens H. Hellmann, Pia Wingbermühle, Nexhmedin Morina, Which psychological characteristics influence therapists' readiness to work with refugees?, Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 10.1002/cpp.2508, 0, 0, (2020).
- Giovanni Piumatti, Marco Salvati, Contact With Gay Men and Lesbian Women Moderates the Negative Relationship Between Religiosity and Endorsement of Same-Sex Unions’ and Families’ Rights, Social Psychology, 10.1027/1864-9335/a000416, (1-10), (2020).
- Karien Dekker, Who is Islamophobic and why? Explanations of Islamophobia in two suburbs with high Muslim concentrations in Melbourne’s north, Australian Geographer, 10.1080/00049182.2020.1840715, (1-18), (2020).
- Jens Peter Frølund Thomsen, Jannik Fenger, Nathalie Rüger Jepsen, The Experiential Basis of Social Trust Towards Ethnic Outgroup Members, Social Indicators Research, 10.1007/s11205-020-02526-1, (2020).
- Sabina Lissitsa, Nonna Kushnirovich, Is negative the new positive? Secondary transfer effect of exposure to LGBT portrayals in TV entertainment programs, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12644, 50, 2, (115-130), (2019).
- Katharina Lefringhausen, Nelli Ferenczi, Tara C. Marshall, Self‐protection and growth as the motivational force behind majority group members' cultural adaptation and discrimination: A parallel mediation model via intergroup contact and threat, International Journal of Psychology, 10.1002/ijop.12620, 55, 4, (532-542), (2019).
- Sebastian Lutterbach, Andreas Beelmann, Positive and negative intergroup contact and shared reality: Contact effects among host society and refugees, European Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1002/ejsp.2601, 50, 1, (143-159), (2019).
- Sofia Stathi, Rita Guerra, Gian Antonio Di Bernardo, Loris Vezzali, Spontaneous imagined contact and intergroup relations: Quality matters, European Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1002/ejsp.2600, 50, 1, (124-142), (2019).
- Tom Nijs, Tobias H. Stark, Maykel Verkuyten, Negative Intergroup Contact and Radical Right‐Wing Voting: The Moderating Roles of Personal and Collective Self‐Efficacy, Political Psychology, 10.1111/pops.12577, 40, 5, (1057-1073), (2019).
- Rodrigo Lagos, Enrique Canessa, Sergio E. Chaigneau, Modeling stereotypes and negative self‐stereotypes as a function of interactions among groups with power asymmetries, Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 10.1111/jtsb.12207, 49, 3, (312-333), (2019).
- Tibor Zingora, Sylvie Graf, Marry who you love: Intergroup contact with gay people and another stigmatized minority is related to voting on the restriction of gay rights through threat, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12627, 49, 11, (684-703), (2019).
- Rose Meleady, Marieke Vermue, The effect of intergroup contact on solidarity‐based collective action is mediated by reductions in SDO, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12586, 49, 5, (307-318), (2019).
- Changcheng Wang, Fei Huang, Loris Vezzali, A test of positive and negative extended intergroup contact in a Chinese minority with perceived group norms and intergroup anxiety as mediators, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12532, 49, 7, (399-408), (2019).
- Rupert Brown, Sam Pehrson, Bringing Groups Together, Group Processes ‐ Dynamics within and Between Groups, 10.1002/9781118719244, (261-307), (2019).
- Sabahat Cigdem Bagci, Hazal Gungor, Associations between perceived positive and negative parental contact and adolescents’ intergroup contact experiences, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2019.01.002, 69, (76-86), (2019).
- Jasper Van Assche, Ethnic Diversity, Ideological Climates, and Intergroup Relations: A Person × Context Approach, Psychologica Belgica, 10.5334/pb.465, 59, 1, (33-49), (2019).
- Jason Shuo Zhang, Chenhao Tan, Qin Lv, Intergroup Contact in the Wild, Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction, 10.1145/3359295, 3, CSCW, (1-35), (2019).
- Fiona Kate Barlow, Matthew J. Hornsey, Lydia E. Hayward, Carla A. Houkamau, Jemima Kang, Petar Milojev, Chris G. Sibley, Why Do We Hold Mixed Emotions About Racial Out-Groups? A Case for Affect Matching, Psychological Science, 10.1177/0956797619844269, (095679761984426), (2019).
- Brian A. O’Shea, Derrick G. Watson, Gordon D. A. Brown, Corey L. Fincher, Infectious Disease Prevalence, Not Race Exposure, Predicts Both Implicit and Explicit Racial Prejudice Across the United States, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10.1177/1948550619862319, (194855061986231), (2019).
- Ariana N. Bell, Danielle S. Smith, Jaana Juvonen, Interpersonal attitudes toward cross-ethnic peers in diverse middle schools: Implications for intergroup attitudes, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430219888020, (136843021988802), (2019).
- Christian Schemer, Christine E. Meltzer, The Impact of Negative Parasocial and Vicarious Contact with Refugees in the Media on Attitudes toward Refugees, Mass Communication and Society, 10.1080/15205436.2019.1692037, (1-19), (2019).
- Magdalena Wojcieszak, Nuri Kim, Juan-José Igartua, How to Enhance the Effects of Mediated Intergroup Contact? Evidence from Four Countries, Mass Communication and Society, 10.1080/15205436.2019.1630444, (2019).
- Lisbeth Drury, Giovanni A. Travaglino, Demobilising by legitimising: Masculine honour, positive and negative contact, and social activism against criminal organisations, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430219842917, (136843021984291), (2019).
- Marieke Vermue, Rose Meleady, Charles R. Seger, Member-to-member generalisation in trust behaviour: How do prior experiences inform prosocial behaviour towards novel ingroup and outgroup members?, Current Psychology, 10.1007/s12144-019-00289-8, (2019).
- Bo-Kyung Byun, Hyejoo Jung, From policy rhetoric to reality: the experiences of Southeast Asian science and engineering graduate students in Korea, Asia Pacific Education Review, 10.1007/s12564-019-09588-y, (2019).
- Matt Motyl, J. P. Prims, Ravi Iyer, How Ambient Cues Facilitate Political Segregation, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10.1177/0146167219875141, (014616721987514), (2019).
- Susanne Bygnes, A collective sigh of relief: Local reactions to the establishment of new asylum centers in Norway, Acta Sociologica, 10.1177/0001699319833143, (000169931983314), (2019).
- Mirjana Rupar, Sylvie Graf, Different forms of intergroup contact with former adversary are linked to distinct reconciliatory acts through symbolic and realistic threat, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12565, 49, 2, (63-74), (2018).
- Rose Meleady, Laura Forder, When contact goes wrong: Negative intergroup contact promotes generalized outgroup avoidance, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430218761568, 22, 5, (688-707), (2018).
- Shenel Husnu, Stefania Paolini, Positive imagined contact is actively chosen: Exploring determinants and consequences of volitional intergroup imagery in a conflict-ridden setting, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430217747405, 22, 4, (511-529), (2018).
- Stefania Paolini, Kylie McIntyre, Bad Is Stronger Than Good for Stigmatized, but Not Admired Outgroups: Meta-Analytical Tests of Intergroup Valence Asymmetry in Individual-to-Group Generalization Experiments, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10.1177/1088868317753504, 23, 1, (3-47), (2018).
- Stefania Paolini, Jake Harwood, Miles Hewstone, David L. Neumann, Seeking and avoiding intergroup contact: Future frontiers of research on building social integration, Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10.1111/spc3.12422, 12, 12, (2018).
- Nadia P. Andrews, Kumar Yogeeswaran, Matt J. Walker, Miles Hewstone, Effect of valenced vicarious online contact on out‐group prejudice and perceived out‐group variability: A study of online poker, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12548, 48, 10, (571-581), (2018).
- Katrín Árnadóttir, Simon Lolliot, Rupert Brown, Miles Hewstone, Positive and negative intergroup contact: Interaction not asymmetry, European Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1002/ejsp.2365, 48, 6, (784-800), (2018).
- Linda R. Tropp, Dina G. Okamoto, Helen B. Marrow, Michael Jones-Correa, How Contact Experiences Shape Welcoming: Perspectives from U.S.-Born and Immigrant Groups, Social Psychology Quarterly, 10.1177/0190272517747265, 81, 1, (23-47), (2018).
- Nuri Kim, Magdalena Wojcieszak, Intergroup contact through online comments: Effects of direct and extended contact on outgroup attitudes, Computers in Human Behavior, 10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.013, 81, (63-72), (2018).
- Roger M. Whitaker, Gualtiero B. Colombo, David G. Rand, Indirect Reciprocity and the Evolution of Prejudicial Groups, Scientific Reports, 10.1038/s41598-018-31363-z, 8, 1, (2018).
- Muqtafi Akhmad, Shuang Chang, Hiroshi Deguchi, undefined, 2018 Joint 10th International Conference on Soft Computing and Intelligent Systems (SCIS) and 19th International Symposium on Advanced Intelligent Systems (ISIS), 10.1109/SCIS-ISIS.2018.00202, (1289-1294), (2018).
- Sabina Lissitsa, Nonna Kushnirovich, Secondary transfer effect of positive and negative online contact between groups involved in high-intensity conflict, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2018.10.001, 67, (71-80), (2018).
- Hans Spijkerman, Yvonne W.M. Benschop, Joost Bücker, Constructive intercultural contact: yes we can. Introduction of a new concept, Equality, Diversity and Inclusion: An International Journal, 10.1108/EDI-05-2016-0042, 37, 7, (649-663), (2018).
- Shervin Assari, Does School Racial Composition Explain Why High Income Black Youth Perceive More Discrimination? A Gender Analysis, Brain Sciences, 10.3390/brainsci8080140, 8, 8, (140), (2018).
- Sylvie Graf, Stefania Paolini, Mark Rubin, Does intimacy counteract or amplify the detrimental effects of negative intergroup contact on attitudes?, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430218767026, (136843021876702), (2018).
- Erik Bleich, James P Callison, Georgia Grace Edwards, Mia Fichman, Erin Hoynes, Razan Jabari, A Maurits van der Veen, The good, the bad, and the ugly: A corpus linguistics analysis of US newspaper coverage of Latinx, 1996–2016, Journalism, 10.1177/1464884918818252, (146488491881825), (2018).
- Sabahat C. Bagci, Abbas Turnuklu, Intended, Unintended, and Unknown Consequences of Contact, Social Psychology, 10.1027/1864-9335/a000355, (1-17), (2018).
- Şenay Yitmen, Maykel Verkuyten, Feelings toward refugees and non‐Muslims in Turkey: The roles of national and religious identifications, and multiculturalism, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12493, 48, 2, (90-100), (2017).
- Lydia E. Hayward, Linda R. Tropp, Matthew J. Hornsey, Fiona Kate Barlow, How negative contact and positive contact with Whites predict collective action among racial and ethnic minorities, British Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1111/bjso.12220, 57, 1, (1-20), (2017).
- Sara L. Buckingham, Lindsay Emery, Surbhi Godsay, Anne E. Brodsky, Jill E. Scheibler, ‘You opened my mind’: Latinx immigrant and receiving community interactional dynamics in the United States, Journal of Community Psychology, 10.1002/jcop.21931, 46, 2, (171-186), (2017).
- Hema Preya Selvanathan, Pirathat Techakesari, Linda R. Tropp, Fiona Kate Barlow, Whites for racial justice: How contact with Black Americans predicts support for collective action among White Americans, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430217690908, 21, 6, (893-912), (2017).
- Shelley McKeown, John Dixon, The “contact hypothesis”: Critical reflections and future directions, Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10.1111/spc3.12295, 11, 1, (2017).
- Lydia E. Hayward, Matthew J. Hornsey, Linda R. Tropp, Fiona Kate Barlow, Positive and negative intergroup contact predict Black and White Americans' judgments about police violence against Black Americans, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12463, 47, 11, (605-615), (2017).
- Rose Meleady, Charles R. Seger, Marieke Vermue, Examining the role of positive and negative intergroup contact and anti‐immigrant prejudice in Brexit, British Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1111/bjso.12203, 56, 4, (799-808), (2017).
- Ralf Wölfer, Miles Hewstone, Beyond the dyadic perspective: 10 Reasons for using social network analysis in intergroup contact research, British Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1111/bjso.12195, 56, 3, (609-617), (2017).
- Emilio Paolo Visintin, Eva G.T. Green, Adrienne Pereira, Polimira Miteva, How positive and negative contact relate to attitudes towards Roma: Comparing majority and high‐status minority perspectives, Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 10.1002/casp.2309, 27, 3, (240-252), (2017).
- Jannes Beer ten Berge, Bram Lancee, Eva Jaspers, Can Interethnic Friends Buffer for the Prejudice Increasing Effect of Negative Interethnic Contact? A Longitudinal Study of Adolescents in the Netherlands, European Sociological Review, 10.1093/esr/jcx045, 33, 3, (423-435), (2017).
- Ralf Wölfer, Eva Jaspers, Danielle Blaylock, Clarissa Wigoder, Joanne Hughes, Miles Hewstone, Studying Positive and Negative Direct and Extended Contact: Complementing Self-Reports With Social Network Analysis, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10.1177/0146167217719732, 43, 11, (1566-1581), (2017).
- Lydia E. Hayward, Linda R. Tropp, Matthew J. Hornsey, Fiona Kate Barlow, Toward a Comprehensive Understanding of Intergroup Contact, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10.1177/0146167216685291, 43, 3, (347-364), (2017).
- Gordana Abramovic, Laura E. Mercer Traavik, Support for diversity practices in Norway: Depends on who you are and whom you have met, European Management Journal, 10.1016/j.emj.2017.04.002, 35, 4, (454-463), (2017).
- John F. Dovidio, Angelika Love, Fabian M. H. Schellhaas, Miles Hewstone, Reducing intergroup bias through intergroup contact: Twenty years of progress and future directions, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10.1177/1368430217712052, 20, 5, (606-620), (2017).
- Katy Greenland, Dimitrios Xenias, Gregory R. Maio, Effects of Promotion and Compunction Interventions on Real Intergroup Interactions: Promotion Helps but High Compunction Hurts, Frontiers in Psychology, 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00528, 8, (2017).
- Cécile Rousseau, Youssef Oulhote, Mónica Ruiz-Casares, Janet Cleveland, Christina Greenaway, Encouraging understanding or increasing prejudices: A cross-sectional survey of institutional influence on health personnel attitudes about refugee claimants' access to health care, PLOS ONE, 10.1371/journal.pone.0170910, 12, 2, (e0170910), (2017).
- Jens Peter Frølund Thomsen, Arzoo Rafiqi, When does superficial intergroup contact reduce anti-foreigner sentiment? Negative contact as an essential condition, International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 10.1177/0020715217744598, (002071521774459), (2017).
- Mathias Kauff, Frank Asbrock, Ulrich Wagner, Thomas F. Pettigrew, Miles Hewstone, Sarina J. Schäfer, Oliver Christ, (Bad) Feelings about Meeting Them? Episodic and Chronic Intergroup Emotions Associated with Positive and Negative Intergroup Contact As Predictors of Intergroup Behavior, Frontiers in Psychology, 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01449, 8, (2017).
- Nils Karl Reimer, Julia C. Becker, Angelika Benz, Oliver Christ, Kristof Dhont, Ulrich Klocke, Sybille Neji, Magdalena Rychlowska, Katharina Schmid, Miles Hewstone, Intergroup Contact and Social Change, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10.1177/0146167216676478, 43, 1, (121-136), (2016).
- Emilio Paolo Visintin, Alberto Voci, Lisa Pagotto, Miles Hewstone, Direct, extended, and mass‐mediated contact with immigrants in Italy: their associations with emotions, prejudice, and humanity perceptions, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12423, 47, 4, (175-194), (2016).
- Lisbeth Drury, Dominic Abrams, Hannah J. Swift, Ruth A. Lamont, Katarina Gerocova, Can Caring Create Prejudice? An Investigation of Positive and Negative Intergenerational Contact in Care Settings and the Generalisation of Blatant and Subtle Age Prejudice to Other Older People, Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 10.1002/casp.2294, 27, 1, (65-82), (2016).
- Vasile Cernat, Trajectories of interethnic contact and prejudice in a four-waves study of Romanian teenager–parent pairs, Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 10.1080/1369183X.2016.1271699, 43, 15, (2669-2687), (2016).
- Loris Vezzali, Luca Andrighetto, Gian Antonio Di Bernardo, Chaima Nadi, Giulia Bergamini, Negative intergroup contact and support for social policies toward the minority outgroup in the aftermath of a natural disaster, The Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1080/00224545.2016.1184126, 157, 4, (407-415), (2016).
- Ludwin E Molina, Linda R Tropp, Chris Goode, Reflections on prejudice and intergroup relations, Current Opinion in Psychology, 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.08.001, 11, (120-124), (2016).
- Asteria Brylka, Inga Jasinskaja-Lahti, Tuuli Anna Mähönen, The majority influence on interminority attitudes: The secondary transfer effect of positive and negative contact, International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2015.12.007, 50, (76-88), (2016).
- Kylie McIntyre, Stefania Paolini, Miles Hewstone, Changing people’s views of outgroups through individual-to-group generalisation: meta-analytic reviews and theoretical considerations, European Review of Social Psychology, 10.1080/10463283.2016.1201893, 27, 1, (63-115), (2016).
- Magdalena Wojcieszak, Rachid Azrout, I Saw You in the News: Mediated and Direct Intergroup Contact Improve Outgroup Attitudes, Journal of Communication, 10.1111/jcom.12266, 66, 6, (1032-1060), (2016).
- Barbara Lášticová, Andrej Findor, Developing explicit measures of stereotypes and anti-Roma prejudice in Slovakia: Conceptual and methodological challenges, Human Affairs, 10.1515/humaff-2016-0022, 26, 3, (2016).
- Giulio Boccato, Dora Capozza, Elena Trifiletti, Gian Antonio Di Bernardo, Attachment security and intergroup contact, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10.1111/jasp.12325, 45, 11, (629-647), (2015).
- Agostino Mazziotta, Anette Rohmann, Stephen C. Wright, Pablo De Tezanos‐Pinto, Sebastian Lutterbach, (How) does positive and negative extended cross‐group contact predict direct cross‐group contact and intergroup attitudes?, European Journal of Social Psychology, 10.1002/ejsp.2110, 45, 5, (653-667), (2015).
- Miguel R. Ramos, Miles Hewstone, The Good, the Bad, and the Long‐Term Implications of Social Diversity, Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences, 10.1002/9781118900772, (1-14), (2015).
- James R. Rae, Anna-Kaisa Newheiser, Kristina R. Olson, Exposure to Racial Out-Groups and Implicit Race Bias in the United States, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10.1177/1948550614567357, 6, 5, (535-543), (2015).
- Anne Pedersen, Lisa K. Hartley, Can We Make a Difference? Prejudice Towards Asylum Seekers in Australia and the Effectiveness of Antiprejudice Interventions, Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 10.1017/prp.2015.1, 9, 01, (1-14), (2015).
- Mathias Kauff, Marta Beneda, Stefania Paolini, Michał Bilewicz, Patrick Kotzur, Alexander W. O'Donnell, Clifford Stevenson, Ulrich Wagner, Oliver Christ, How do we get people into contact? Predictors of intergroup contact and drivers of contact seeking, Journal of Social Issues, 10.1111/josi.12398, 0, 0, (undefined).